Next: 2 Genotype-Environment Correlation.
Up: 3 Genotype-Environment Effects
Previous: 3 Genotype-Environment Effects
  Index
1 Assortative Mating.
Any non-random pairing of mates on the
basis of factors other than biological relatedness is subsumed under
the general category of assortative mating. Mating based on
relatedness is termed inbreeding, and will not
be examined in
this book.
We discuss assortative mating under the general heading of
genotype-environmental effects for two main reasons. First, when
assortment is based on some aspect of the phenotype, it may be
influenced by both genetic and environmental factors. Second,
assortative mating may affect the transmission, magnitude, and
correlation of both genetic and environmental effects.
In human populations, the first indication of assortative mating
is often a correlation between the phenotypes of mates. Usually, such
correlations are positive. Positive assortment is most marked for
traits in the domains of education, religion, attitudes, and
socioeconomic status. Somewhat smaller correlations are found in the
physical and cognitive domains. Mating is effectively random, or only
very slightly assortative, in the personality domain. We are not
aware of any replicated finding of a significant negative husband-wife
correlation, with the exception of gender!
Assortative mating may not be the sole source of similarity
between husband and wife -- social interaction
is another plausible cause. A priori, we might expect
social interaction to play a particularly important role in
spousal resemblance for habits such as cigarette smoking and
alcohol consumption. Two approaches are available for resolving
spousal interaction from strict assortative mating. The first
depends on tracing the change in spousal resemblance over time,
and the second requires analyzing the resemblance between the
spouses of biologically related individuals (see Heath, 1987).
Although the usual treatment of assortative mating assumes
that spouses choose one another on the basis of the trait being
studied (primary phenotypic assortment), we should understand
that this is only one model of a process that might
be
more
complicated in reality.
For example, mate selection is unlikely to be based on an actual
psychological test score. Instead it is probably based on
some related variable, which may or may not be measured directly.
If the variable on which selection is based is something that
we have also measured, we call it correlated variable
assortment. If the correlated trait
is
not
measured directly we have latent variable assortment. In
the
simplest case, the latent variable may simply be the true
value of trait of which the actual measure is just a more or
less unreliable index. We then speak of phenotypic assortment
with error.
Once we begin to consider latent variable assortment, we
recognize that the latent variable may be more or less genetic.
If the latent variable is due entirely to the social environment
we have one form of social homogamy (e.g., Rao et al.,
1974). We can conceive
of a number of intriguing mechanisms of latent variable assortment according
to the presumed causes of the latent variable on which mate selection
is based.
For example, mating may be based on one or more aspects of the phenotypes of
relatives, such as parents' incomes, culinary skills, or siblings'
reproductive history. In all these cases of correlated or latent variable
assortment, mate selection may be based on variables that are more reliable
indices of the genotype than the measured phenotype. This possibility was
considered by Fisher (1918) in what is still
the classical treatment of
assortative mating.
Clearly, the resolution of these various mechanisms of
assortment is beyond the scope of the conventional twin study,
although multivariate studies that include the spouses of twins,
or the parents and parents-in-law of twins may be capable of
resolving some of these complex issues (see, e.g., Heath et al.,
1985).
Even though the classical twin study cannot resolve the
complexities of mate selection, we have to keep the issue in mind
all the time because of the effects of assortment on the
correlations between relatives, including twins. When mates
select partners like themselves phenotypically, they are also
(indirectly) choosing people who resemble themselves genetically and
culturally. As a result, positive phenotypic assortative mating increases
the genetic and environmental correlations between relatives.
Translating this principle into the context of the twin study, we
will find that assortative mating tends to increase the
similarity of DZ twins relative to MZ twins. As we shall see, in
twins reared together, the genetic effects of assortative mating
will artificially inflate estimates of the family environmental component.
This means, in turn, that estimates of the genetic component
based primarily on the difference between MZ correlations and DZ
correlations
will tend to be biased downwards in the presence of assortative
mating.
Next: 2 Genotype-Environment Correlation.
Up: 3 Genotype-Environment Effects
Previous: 3 Genotype-Environment Effects
  Index
Jeff Lessem
2002-03-21