Next: 5 Relationships between Variables
Up: 3 Genotype-Environment Effects
Previous: 2 Genotype-Environment Correlation.
  Index
The interaction of genotype and environment (``G E'') must
always
be distinguished carefully from CorGE. Genotype-environment
correlation reflects a non-random distribution of environments among
different genotypes. ``Good'' genotypes get more or less than their
fair share of ``good'' environments. By contrast, G E
interaction has nothing to do with the distribution of genetic and
environmental effects. Instead, it relates to the actual way genes
and environment affect the phenotype. G E refers to the
genetic control of sensitivity to differences in the environment. The
old adage ``sauce for the goose is sauce for the gander'' describes a
world in which G E is absent, because it implies that the same
environmental treatment has the same positive or negative effect
regardless of the genotype of the individual upon whom it is imposed.
An obvious example of G E interaction is that of
inherited
disease resistance. Genetically susceptible individuals will be
free of disease as long as the environment does not contain the
pathogen. Resistant individuals will be free of the disease even
in a pathogenic environment. That is, changing the environment
by introducing the pathogen will have quite a different impact on
the phenotype of susceptible individuals than on
resistant ones. More subtle examples may be the genetic
control of sensitivity to the pathogenic effects of tobacco smoke
or genetic differences in the effects of sodium intake on blood
pressure.
The analysis of G E in humans is extremely difficult in
practice because of the difficulty of securing large enough
samples to detect effects that may be small compared with the
main effects of genes and environment. Studies of G E in
experimental organisms (see, e.g., Mather and Jinks, 1982)
illustrate a number of issues which are also conceptually
important in thinking about GE in humans. We consider
these briefly in turn.
The genes responsible for sensitivity to the environment are
not always the same as those that control average trait values.
For example, one set of genes may control overall liability to
depression and a second set, quite distinct in their location and
mode of action, may control whether individuals respond more or
less to stressful environments. Another way of thinking about
the issue is to consider measurements made in different
environments as different traits which may or may not be controlled
by the same genes. By analogy with our earlier discussion of
sex-limitation, we distinguish between ``scalar'' and ``non-scalar''
G E interaction. When the same genes are expressed
consistently
at
all levels of a salient environmental variable so that only
the amount of genetic variance changes between environments, we
have ``scalar genotype environment interaction.'' If,
instead
of, or in addition to, changes in genetic variance, we also find
that different genes are expressed in different environments we
have ``non-scalar G E.''
G E interaction may involve environments that can be
measured directly or whose effects can be
inferred only from the correlations between relatives. Generally, our
chances of detecting G E are much greater when we can
measure the
relevant environments, such as diet, stress, or tobacco
consumption. The simplest situation, which we shall discuss in
Chapter 9, arises when each individual in a twin pair can be
scored for the presence or absence of a particular environmental
variable such as exposure to severe psychological stress. In
this case, twin pairs can be divided into those who are
concordant and those discordant for environmental exposure and the data
can be tested for different kinds of G E using relatively
simple methods.
One ``measurable'' feature of the environment may be the
phenotype of an individual's parent. A problem frequently
encountered, however, is the fact that many measurable aspects
of the environment, such as smoking and alcohol consumption,
themselves have a genetic component so that the problems of
mathematical modelling and statistical analysis become
formidable. If we are unable to measure the environment
directly, our ability to detect and analyze G E will depend
on
the same kinds of data that we use to analyze the main effects of
genes and environment, namely the patterns of family resemblance
and other, more complex, features of the distribution of trait
values in families. Generally, the detection of any interaction
between genetic effects and unmeasured aspects of the
between-family environment will require adoption data, particularly
separated MZ twins. Interaction between genes and the
within-family environment will usually be detectable only if the genes
controlling sensitivity are correlated with those controlling
average expression of the trait (see, e.g., Jinks and Fulker,
1970).
Next: 5 Relationships between Variables
Up: 3 Genotype-Environment Effects
Previous: 2 Genotype-Environment Correlation.
  Index
Jeff Lessem
2002-03-21